Teachionary, a language-learning program he developed. Heâs since tried other jobs: construction manager, carpenter, pizza delivery guy, plumberâs helper.
Veatchâs tale seems like a testament to just how daunting a task it is to define humor once and for all. But his predecessorâs fate hardly gave Pete pause. Veatchâs theory engrossed him. As far as he could tell, Veatch had nearly hit the theoretical bullâs-eye. But something about it still seemed not quite right.
Peteâs department chair, Donnie Lichtenstein, summed up the problem when doctoral student Caleb Warren tried to illustrate Veatchâs theory by referring to a fictional story used in psychological surveys that often got people chuckling. As the tale goes, a man decides to use his kitten as a sex toy, with the feline purring in enjoyment. That situation may be funny, said Lichtenstein, but nothing about it is normal.
So Pete and Caleb set upon improving Veatchâs work and ended up with a new comedic axiom: the benign violation theory. Accordingto this amended theory, humor only occurs when something seems wrong, unsettling or threatening (i.e., a violation), but simultaneously seems okay, acceptable, or safe (i.e., benign). When something is just a violation, such as somebody falling down the stairs, people feel bad about it. But according to Pete and Caleb, when the violation turns out to be benign, such as someone falling down the stairs and ending up unhurt, people often do an about-face and react in at least one of three ways: they feel amused, they laugh, or they make a judgmentââThat was funny.â
To them, the term âbenign,â rather than ânormal,â better encapsulated the many ways a violation could be okay, acceptable, or safeâand gave them a clear-cut tool to determine when and why a violation such as the feline-turned-sex-toy story can be funny. While heavy petting with a kitten may not be normal, according to the story, the kitten purred and seemed to enjoy the contact. The violation was benignâno kittens were harmed in the making of the joke. Later, when Pete and Caleb used this story in an experiment, participants who read a version in which the kitten whined in displeasure at the heavy petting found the tale far less funny than the âhappy kittyâ scenario. 8
Then thereâs the story of the church-raffle Hummer that got Pete pondering what makes things funny in the first place. The idea of mixing the sanctity of Christianity with a four-wheeled symbol of secular excess strikes people as a violation. But when Pete presented the raffle story to regular churchgoers as well as people who rarely go to church, those less committed to Christianity were more likely to find a holy Hummer benign and therefore found it funnier. 9
Immoral behaviors are not the only kind of humorous situation that could be explained by the benign violation theory. A dirty joke trades on moral or social violations, but itâs only going to get a laugh if the person listening is liberated enough to consider risqué subjects such as sex okay to talk about. Puns can be seen as linguistic violations that still make grammatical sense, though theyâre typically only funny to cerebral types and grammarians who care about the nuances of the English language. Sarcasm violates conversational rules by meaning the opposite of whatâs said. No one is going to be amused by a crack like âYouâre good at basketball? Yeah, right!â if they donât notice the exaggerated tone and grasp the intended meaning. Nor is the guy who thinks heâs good at basketball.
And tickling, long a sticking point for other humor theories, fits perfectly. After all, tickling involves violating someoneâs physical space in a benign way. People canât tickle themselvesâa phenomenon that baffled Aristotleâbecause it isnât a violation. Nor will people laugh if a creepy